Sexual Selection and Neolithic Monument Building
Also available for download at https://www.researchgate.net/publication/391279201_Sexual_Selection_and_Neolithic_Monument_Building/references
Tim Daw - Cannings Cross, SN10 3NP
Open Access - 29th April 2025
Abstract
This paper proposes that Neolithic monuments in Britain, such as Stonehenge, Avebury, and Maeshowe, may have functioned as displays driven by sexual selection, akin to a peacock's tail or a bowerbird's bower. Extending a framework developed for Acheulean handaxes, we argue that these monuments signalled cognitive ability, health management, resource access, and social status through an instinctive mate-driven selection process. Their scale, engineering complexity, visibility, and permanence served as enduring symbols of group fitness, potentially influencing mate choice and social alliances. The Red Queen hypothesis is invoked to suggest an evolutionary arms race, where groups escalated monument building to outcompete rivals in reproductive and social status, rather than for purely utilitarian purposes. Whilst acknowledging ritual, cosmological, and social functions, we propose that the display value may have been a significant evolutionary driver, offering a novel perspective on Neolithic social dynamics.
Introduction
The Late Neolithic and Early Bronze Age in Britain (circa 3000-2500 BCE) marked a period of extraordinary monument building, producing iconic structures such as Stonehenge, Silbury Hill, Avebury, Durrington Walls, the Ring of Brodgar, Flagstones Enclosure, and Maeshowe. Traditionally, these monuments have been interpreted as ritualistic, cosmological, or communal in purpose, serving as ceremonial sites, astronomical observatories, or gathering places for dispersed populations (Darvill, 2006; Parker Pearson, 2012). However, a provocative hypothesis inspired by evolutionary biology suggests that sexual selection may have played a pivotal role in their construction, drawing parallels with the Acheulean handaxe tradition, where tools may have served as mate-attracting displays (Kohn & Mithen, 1998). This paper integrates this framework with recent insights on hygiene management (Whitehead et al., 2024), conspicuous consumption (Veblen, 1899), and the Red Queen hypothesis (Van Valen, 1973) to argue that Neolithic monuments functioned as multifaceted displays of skill, health, wealth, and status, influencing mate choice and social dynamics through an instinctive mate-driven selection process. Whilst speculative, this perspective complements traditional interpretations, offering a richer understanding of Neolithic social behaviour.
Theoretical Framework
Sexual Selection and Evolutionary Displays
Sexual selection, as conceptualised by Darwin (1871), operates distinctly from natural selection, driving traits that enhance reproductive success through mate attraction or competition (Darwin, 1871). A classic example is the peacock's tail, which, despite its survival costs—being heavy and attracting predators—persists because peahens prefer males with the most elaborate displays (Zahavi, 1975). Runaway selection, formalised by Fisher (1930), describes how such traits escalate when mate preferences and the trait co-evolve, creating an evolutionary feedback loop where displays become increasingly exaggerated (Fisher, 1930). The Red Queen hypothesis further contextualises this as an evolutionary arms race, where individuals or groups must continually adapt to maintain relative fitness in a competitive environment (Van Valen, 1973). In human societies, sexual selection can manifest through cultural displays, such as art, architecture, or technology, which signal traits like creativity, leadership, or resource access (Miller, 2000).
Instinctive Mate-Driven Selection
The sexual selection process discussed here is not a conscious strategy but an instinctive mate-driven selection, where behaviours are shaped by subconscious evolutionary pressures. Humans, like other animals, are driven by innate preferences for traits indicating fitness—such as cognitive ability, health, or wealth—without deliberate intent (Ridley, 2025). Over generations, these preferences favoured individuals or groups who exhibited such traits, embedding these behaviours into cultural practices. This clarification ensures that readers do not misinterpret monument building as a deliberate attempt to attract mates, but rather as an emergent outcome of evolutionary dynamics.
Acheulean Handaxes as a Precursor
The Acheulean period (circa 1.76 million to 100,000 years ago) provides a foundational analogy for this hypothesis. Handaxes, often crafted with symmetry beyond functional necessity, may have served as displays of skill and fitness, akin to a bowerbird's bower (Kohn & Mithen, 1998). Experimental studies have shown that symmetrical handaxes required significant skill to produce, suggesting a cognitive and motor prowess that could signal genetic quality (Lycett et al., 2010). Additionally, Whitehead et al. (2024) argue that discarding handaxes after use mitigated pathogen transmission, indicating health awareness—a trait beneficial for survival and offspring care (Whitehead et al., 2024). The resource intensity of crafting and discarding handaxes aligns with conspicuous consumption, where wasteful displays signal wealth and status (Veblen, 1899). These handaxes, often found in large numbers at sites like Boxgrove, may have been prehistoric "status symbols," enhancing the maker's reproductive success through mate choice (Nowell & Chang, 2009).
Neolithic Monuments: A Societal-Scale Display
The Monument-Building Context
The Late Neolithic in Britain was a time of social transformation, marked by the shift to agriculture, population growth, and emerging social hierarchies (Thomas, 2013). Monuments like Stonehenge, constructed in phases from 3000 to 2000 BCE, involved transporting massive sarsen stones from Marlborough Downs, 20 miles away, and bluestones from the Preseli Hills in Wales, over 150 miles (Darvill, 2006). Silbury Hill, built around 2400 BCE, is the largest prehistoric man-made mound in Europe, requiring an estimated 4 million labour hours (Whittle, 1997). Avebury, dating to around 2600 BCE, features the largest stone circle in Europe, enclosing two smaller circles. Durrington Walls, a henge near Stonehenge from 2500 BCE, housed temporary settlements for builders. The Ring of Brodgar in Orkney, also around 2500 BCE, is part of a broader ritual landscape. The Flagstones Enclosure, a Neolithic ditched enclosure in Dorchester from around 3200 BCE, and Maeshowe, a passage grave in Orkney dated to 2800 BCE, further exemplify this monumental tradition (Henshall, 1972). These projects coincided with increased trade networks, as evidenced by the distribution of jadeite axes from the Alps, and social stratification, seen in elite burials like the Amesbury Archer near Stonehenge (Fitzpatrick, 2011).
Skill and Cognitive Ability
The construction of Stonehenge required advanced engineering, planning, and coordination, comparable to the skill needed for handaxe crafting. The precise alignment of Stonehenge's stones with the solstices demonstrates astronomical knowledge, whilst the structural stability of Maeshowe's corbelled chamber reflects sophisticated masonry (Darvill, 2006; Henshall, 1972). These feats signal cognitive ability and leadership, traits valued in mate choice across species (Miller, 2000). The complexity of Avebury's concentric circles and the Ring of Brodgar's geometric precision further underscore this, suggesting that groups capable of such achievements were perceived as fit and capable partners or allies (Burl, 2000).
Health Management and Communal Gatherings
Neolithic monuments often served as gathering places, as evidenced by feasting debris at Durrington Walls, where thousands of animal bones and pottery fragments indicate large-scale events (Parker Pearson, 2012). Managing such gatherings without health crises—such as starvation or food poisoning—required significant organisational skills, akin to modern public health measures. This ability to ensure group health parallels the hygiene benefits of discarding handaxes, as proposed by Whitehead et al. (2024), and could enhance a group's or leader's attractiveness by demonstrating care and reliability (Whitehead et al., 2024).
Wealth and Conspicuous Consumption
The resource intensity of these monuments reflects conspicuous consumption on a societal scale. As examples Silbury Hill's construction involved moving vast quantities of chalk, whilst Stonehenge's bluestones required long-distance transport, both demanding significant labour and materials (Whittle, 1997). These displays of wealth and power, as Veblen (1899) describes, could attract mates or allies by signalling resource access and group cohesion, enhancing reproductive success through social alliances (Veblen, 1899).
Visibility and Permanence as Status Symbols
Unlike portable handaxes, Neolithic monuments were permanent and highly visible. Stonehenge, standing on Salisbury Plain, can be seen from miles away, whilst Avebury's massive circle dominates its landscape (Darvill, 2006). The Ring of Brodgar, set within Orkney's dramatic scenery, and Maeshowe's prominent mound served as enduring symbols of group status (Burl, 2000). This permanence acted as a "prestige billboard," broadcasting fitness traits across generations, much like a peacock's tail or a modern landmark (Zahavi, 1975).
Sexual Selection Dynamics
Instinctive Mate-Driven Selection
The sexual selection process here is an instinctive mate-driven selection, where mate choice operates subconsciously, driven by innate preferences for fitness traits. Neolithic individuals did not deliberately build monuments to attract mates; rather, their behaviours were shaped by evolutionary pressures favouring groups that displayed skill, health, wealth, and status (Ridley, 2025). This mirrors how female bowerbirds instinctively prefer males with the most elaborate bowers, a behaviour that enhances reproductive success without conscious intent (Diamond, 1986).
Mate Choice and Social Alliances
In Neolithic societies, where survival and reproduction were paramount, traits like cognitive ability, health management, and resource access were likely valued in mates. Leaders or groups orchestrating monumental projects would have been perceived as desirable partners, enhancing their reproductive success (Kohn & Mithen, 1998). Evidence of inter-group interactions, such as the Amesbury Archer's isotopic signature indicating origins in the Alps, suggests mate exchange between communities, where monument-building groups may have gained a reproductive edge (Fitzpatrick, 2011). Additionally, monuments could attract allies, strengthening social networks and indirectly boosting mating opportunities (Parker Pearson, 2012).
Runaway Selection and the Red Queen
The competition to build larger or more impressive monuments reflects a runaway selection process, where groups escalated displays to outdo rivals, driven by social and reproductive pressures (Fisher, 1930). The Red Queen hypothesis frames this as an evolutionary arms race, where groups had to continually innovate to maintain relative fitness (Van Valen, 1973). The clustering of monuments in regions like Wiltshire (Stonehenge, Avebury, Silbury Hill) and Orkney (Ring of Brodgar, Maeshowe) suggests such a race, with groups vying to assert dominance through ever-grander displays (Burl, 2000).
Supporting Evidence and Counterarguments
Supporting Evidence
- Archaeological Findings: Feasting at Durrington Walls, with thousands of animal bones and pottery fragments, indicates large gatherings, likely venues for social interaction and mate choice (Parker Pearson, 2012). Similar evidence at Maeshowe, where communal rituals involved feasting, supports this (Henshall, 1972).
- Monument Complexity: The scale of Stonehenge, the geometric precision of the Ring of Brodgar, and the labour-intensive construction of Silbury Hill indicate significant resource investment, consistent with conspicuous consumption (Whittle, 1997).
- Symbolic Artefacts: Elaborate pottery, such as Grooved Ware at Durrington Walls, and jadeite axes from the Alps found across Britain, further suggest the importance of status displays in Neolithic societies (Thomas, 2013).
- Evolutionary Parallels: In other species, such as bowerbirds, elaborate displays are driven by sexual selection, supporting the analogy with human monuments (Diamond, 1986). The peacock's tail, a product of runaway selection, provides a direct parallel to Stonehenge's grandeur (Zahavi, 1975).
Counterarguments
- Ritual and Cosmological Functions: Stonehenge's solstice alignments and Maeshowe's winter solstice orientation suggest primary roles as ritual or astronomical sites (Darvill, 2006; Henshall, 1972). These functions may overshadow social displays.
- Egalitarian Societies: Some scholars argue that Neolithic societies were egalitarian, reducing the role of status competition (Whittle, 1997). However, the scale of monuments and elite burials, like the Amesbury Archer, imply hierarchy (Fitzpatrick, 2011).
- Lack of Direct Evidence: The sexual selection hypothesis lacks direct evidence, relying on analogies and inference. No isotopic or genetic data confirm mate choice at these sites, and communal health practices are inferred.
Addressing Counterarguments
The sexual selection hypothesis complements rather than negates traditional interpretations. Monuments likely served multiple purposes—ritual, cosmological, and social—with display value enhancing their significance in an evolutionary context. The scale of construction and evidence of hierarchy challenge egalitarian views, whilst the bowerbird analogy provides a robust theoretical framework, even without direct evidence. Future genetic studies could test this hypothesis by examining mating patterns at monument sites.
Application to Other Monuments
This theory extends to other Neolithic monuments across Britain. Avebury's massive stone circle, Silbury Hill's towering mound, and the Ring of Brodgar's remote yet precise construction all reflect significant skill, resources, and labour, potentially serving as displays of group fitness (Burl, 2000). Maeshowe, with its communal burials, and, with its sophisticated passage grave, further illustrate this trend, suggesting that monument building was a widespread strategy for social and reproductive competition in Neolithic Britain (Henshall, 1972). Beyond Britain, structures like Göbekti Tepe in Turkey (circa 9600 BCE), with its intricately carved pillars, may reflect similar dynamics, indicating a broader pattern of display-driven monumentalism in early societies (Schmidt, 2010).
Conclusion
The integration of sexual selection, health management, and conspicuous consumption offers a novel perspective on Neolithic monuments like Stonehenge, Avebury, and Maeshowe. These structures likely functioned as multifaceted displays of cognitive ability, health management, resource access, and social status, influencing mate choice and social alliances through an instinctive mate-driven selection process. The Red Queen hypothesis frames their construction as part of an evolutionary arms race, where groups escalated displays to outcompete rivals, embedding these behaviours in cultural practices. Whilst speculative, this hypothesis enriches our understanding of Neolithic social dynamics, highlighting the interplay of biology, culture, and evolution in shaping human history.
References
- Burl, A. (2000). The Stone Circles of Britain, Ireland, and Brittany. Yale University Press. Available at: https://archive.org/details/stonecirclesofbr0000burl
- Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex. John Murray.
- Darvill, T. (2006). Stonehenge: The Biography of a Landscape. Tempus Publishing. Available at: https://www.thehistorypress.co.uk/publication/stonehenge/9780752433424/
- Diamond, J. (1986). Animal art: Variation in bower decorating style among mate bowerbirds Amblyornis inornatus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 83(9), 3042-3046. Available at: https://www.pnas.org/doi/10.1073/pnas.83.9.3042
- Fisher, R. A. (1930). The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection. Oxford University Press.
- Fitzpatrick, A. P. (2011). The Amesbury Archer and the Boscombe Bowmen: Early Bell Beaker burials at Boscombe Down, Amesbury, Wiltshire. Wessex Archaeology Report 27. Available at: https://www.wessexarch.co.uk/our-work/amesbury-archer-andboscombe-bowmen
- Henshall, A. S. (1972). The Chambered Tombs of Scotland: Volume 2. Edinburgh University Press.
- Kohn, M., & Mithen, S. (1998). Handaxes: Products of sexual selection? Antiquity, 72(276), 518-526.
- Lycett, S. J., von Cramon-Taubadel, N., & Gowlett, J. A. J. (2010). A comparative 3D geometric morphometric analysis of Victoria West cores: Implications for the origins of Levallois technology. Journal of Archaeological Science, 37(5), 1110-1117. Available at: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0305440309004610
- Miller, G. F. (2000). The Mating Mind: How Sexual Choice Shaped the Evolution of Human Nature. Doubleday. Available at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/235362665_The_Mating_Mind_How_Sexual_Choice_Shaped_the_Evolution_of_Human_Nature
- Nowell, A., & Chang, M. L. (2009). The case against sexual selection as an explanation of handaxe morphology. World Archaeology, 41(1), 18-32.
- Parker Pearson, M. (2012). Stonehenge: Exploring the Greatest Stone Age Mystery. Simon & Schuster.
- Ridley, M. (2025). Birds, Sex and Beauty. HarperCollins. Available at: https://books.google.com/books/about/Birds_Sex_and_Beauty.html?id=ZHWOOAEACAAJ
- Schmidt, K. (2010). Göbekli Tepe - the Stone Age Sanctuaries. New results of ongoing excavations with a special focus on sculptures and high reliefs. Documenta Praehistorica, 37, 239-256. Available at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/270030960_Gobekli_Tepe_-The_Stone_Age_Sanctuaries_New_results_of_ongoing_excavations_with_a_special_focus_on_sculptures_and_high_reliefs
- Thomas, J. (2013). The Birth of Neolithic Britain: An Interpretive Account. Oxford University Press. Available at: https://global.oup.com/academic/product/the-birth-of-neolithic-britain-9780199681969
- Van Valen, L. (1973). A new evolutionary law. Evolutionary Theory, 1, 1-30.
- Veblen, T. (1899). The Theory of the Leisure Class. Macmillan.
- Whitehead, A., Sinclair, A., & Scott, C. (2024). Cross-contamination via stone tool use: A pilot study of bifacial butchery tools. EXARC Journal, 2024(2).
- Whittle, A. (1997). Sacred Mound, Holy Rings: Silbury Hill and the West Kennet Palisade Enclosures. Oxbow Books.
- Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection—A selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53(1), 205-214.